Cross-Cultural Issues in
Alaskan Education
Vol. II
SOCIAL CONTROL AND SOCIAL ORGANIZATION
IN AN ALASKAN ATHABASKAN CLASSROOM
Howard Van Ness
University of Alaska, Fairbanks
Introduction
This paper presents a microethnographic analysis of the organization
of behavior as six Koyukon Athabaskan students, one non-Indian student,
and a Koyukon Athabaskan teacher “get ready” for an instructional
activity in a rural Alaskan kindergarten classroom. The analysis of
getting ready as an event in this classroom provides an example of
the ways a Koyukon teacher and her students establish a social organizational
structure to accomplish an instructional activity. Of particular importance
to the analysis are the demonstration and display of the covert and
indirect ways the Indian teacher exercises her classroom authority
as she orchestrates the establishment of the whole groups’ structure
by the class. As the teacher and the students go about getting ready,
they complete certain observable tasks, e.g., some of the students
move their chairs to new locations in the classroom, the teacher distributes
the workbooks containing the material for the coming instructional
activity, and the students find the page in the workbook containing
the exercise to be worked on. The ways in which the teacher and the
students go about accomplishing these tasks also establishes the structural
configuration of the social organization of the class for the coming
instructional activity -- the teacher and the students working through
the workbook exercise as a whole group.
The study was conceived as a
preliminary venture to assess the utility of microethnographic analysis
in providing insight into the workings
of Alaskan “cross-cultural” classrooms. This term is
conventionally used in Alaska to mean classrooms with students of
Eskimo, Indian,
or Aleut descent. The microethnographic analysis began through the
process of video-taping and recording the movements and speech of
the teacher and the kindergarten students. An eight-minute, fourteen-second
sequence was selected for analysis and the teacher-student dialogue
was transcribed and keyed to the video tape. The three segments analyzed
include (1) the transition between the Bilingual lesson and the Reading
lesson, (2) “getting ready” for the Reading lesson, and
(3) the Reading lesson. Each of the student and teacher interactions
was numbered and each of the students has the designator “S” plus
a number. The designator for the teacher is “T”.
The analysis
was organized around the following questions:
- What are socially
significant structural elements or features of the organization
of face-to-face interaction during the segment
- How are these
elements observable in the students’ and teacher’s
behavior
- How are these elements organized together as
a system and how can such a system be analytically comprehended
-
What insights for substance and method in further research are
suggested by this preliminary single case analysis?
Several
assumptions were made in conducting this research. The first is
that the behavior of the teacher and students during
the segment
is organized around participants’ “definition of
the situation,” i.e.,
as the definition of the situation changes during the segment,
the organization of the teacher’s and students’ behavior
also changes (see Goffman, 1964, Kendon, 1975, Erickson, 1975
and Florio, 1976, for discussion of “situations” and “situated
behavior”; see Cazden, 1970, Blom and Gumperz, 1972, for
discussions of “situated verbal behavior”).
The second
assumption made is that the definition of the situation is negotiated
or jointly elaborated by the participants (in this
case the teacher and the students) in a process of continual
monitoring and interpreting of the social environment -- the
setting, the
task, and each other’s behavior -- that seeks discrimination
of the context of social activity (Kendon, 1975; Cicourel, 1974;
Goffman,
1961).
The third assumption made in conducting this research is
that, as members monitor and interpret the social environment
in face-to-face
interaction,
behaviors appropriate to the emerging definition of the situation
are generated by the participants through a process of “tacit
choice-making.” Cicourel
combines the interpretive capability and tacit choice-making
by participants in positing the notion of “interpretive
procedures,” described
as “constitutive of members’ sense of social structure
or social organization” (Cicourel, 1974, p. 44). Interpretive
procedures serve the “executive function” for an
individual in everyday social life in helping him to interpret
the social environment
(including the behavior of others) in negotiating a definition
of the situation, and to select social behavior appropriate to
the emerging definition of the situation from his communicative
repertoire (Cicourel, 1974).
The fourth assumption is that the
corpus of tacit knowledge operated on by an individual’s
interpretive procedures in negotiating the “definition
of the situation” and in generating appropriate
social behavior is culturally organized. Implicit in this assumption
is a “cognitive theory of culture,” discussed by
Goodenough (1964; 1971; 1975) and Spradley (1972). According
to this point of
view, a society’s culture consists of
. . .whatever it is
one has to know or believe in order to operate in a manner acceptable
to its member . . . By this definition,
we should note that culture is not a material phenomenon . .
. it does
not consist
of things, people, behavior or emotions. It is, rather, an organization
of these things. It is the forms of things that people have in
mind, their models for perceiving, relating, and otherwise interpreting
them (Goodenough, 1964, p.36).
The fifth assumption is that the
organization of behavior is emergent in the conjoint social interaction
of the teacher and
the students
as they individually exercise their interpretive procedures.
The teacher and students mutually monitor and interpret the
social environment, including one another’s behavior, in an active
process of achieving or accomplishing the social organization
described and analyzed
in this paper (see Garfinkel, 1967, for discussion of the social
order as members’ accomplishment).
Viewing the classroom
social order during the segment as an interactional accomplishment
by the teacher and students focuses
attention
on both as active agents in the establishment and maintenance
of the
classroom
order. In adopting this perspective, this study is distinguishable
from such efforts as Flanders (1970), those reviewed in Dunkin
and Biddle (1974), Grant and Hennings (1971), and Good and
Brophy (1973),
which focus primarily on the teacher as the active agent in
the classroom social order. This view is also distinguishable
from
the view of the classroom social organization as consisting
of sets or
relationships and attendant rights, obligations, and duties
ascribed to teachers and students by the conventions of the
institution
(cf. Firth, 1954; Goodenough, 1965). Studies such as Waller
(1932), Grambs
(1968), Jackson (1968), and Eddy (1969) focus on the regularities
of these limits or boundaries across classrooms in describing
the classroom (school) as an institution distinct from other
institutions
as arenas
for social life in the society.
Florio reconciles the institutional
order of classrooms with the interactional order of everyday classroom
life by suggesting
that
variability of
behavior is allowed by the institutionally conventional ized
social arrangements: “. . . a teacher can behave in
a variety of ways -- with students, parents, or cohorts --
and
still be a teacher” (Florio,
1976, p. 39). While the organization of the teacher’s
and students’ behavior
is bounded by institutional conventions for appropriate or
expected behavior, these conventions for classroom social
arrangements are mediated by Koyukon Athabaskan knowledge
of appropriate
social
behavior. The Koyukon Athabaskan teacher and students “get
ready” for
an instructional activity in a classroom setting in ways
consistent with Koyukon organization of social behavior in
face-to-face
interaction.
The Exercise of Authority: Indian vs. non-Indian
Styles
Philips (1972), studying non-Indian teachers in Oregon,
reported incongruity between the culturally organized ways authority
is exercised in the
classroom and in the Warm Springs Indian community. Erickson
and Mohatt (1976) considered how a teacher could exercise
authority in ways consistent
with those of the Indian community. “We would expect
more covert ways of exercising social control: less attention
directed to
individual children in the presence of other children as
an ‘audience,’ less
overt monitoring of the classroom behavior of individual
children through verbal and non-verbal sanctions by the
teacher” (p.
9). In analyzing their classroom data, they found consistency
between the theoretically
generated form for the Indian exercise of classroom authority
and the ways the Odawa Indian teacher actually exercised
authority in
her classroom. As the year progressed, they also found
signs of “adaptive
drift” by the non- Indian teacher (who was characterized
as concerned, possessing good intuition and who also benefited
from helpful advice
from his Indian principal) toward the “Indian general
form” in
his exercise of classroom authority.
The study presented
in this paper, following Philips (1972) and Erickson and
Mohatt (1976), pays particular attention
to the
ways the Athabaskan
teacher exercises her authority in the classroom. This
aspect of the inquiry explores the interactional devices
manifested
by the teacher as she exercises her classroom authority
and the degree
of
voluntariness and self-determination in participation allowed
the students by the teacher.
One of the ways teachers on
the Warm Springs Reservation exercised their authority in ways discontinuous
with those
of the Indian
community is in the use, as a pedagogical device, of
placing students in
the position of committing “public mistakes.” Erickson
and Mohatt (1976) investigated “to what extent
the teacher, by questioning, commanding, praising, smiling,
looking pointedly and other
means of directing attention to a specific child, focuses
on what that child is doing in the presence of an ‘audience’ of
other children” (p. 4). Their findings indicate
that the Indian teacher put students in the public “spotlight” much
less than did the non-Indian teacher; additionally the
non-Indian teacher seemed
to make decreasing use of the “spotlight effect” as
the year progressed. The inquiry here combines the insights
of Philips
(1972) and Erickson and Mohatt (1976) in examining the
degree to which the social organization of the lesson
is organized around the teacher’s
placing students “in the spotlight” and the
management of “public mistakes” by the teacher
and the students.
Analysis of the Lesson
In the lesson analyzed in this paper, initiation-reply
exchanges occur throughout the sequence. The initiating
may be done
by the teacher
as in this example:
(21) T: “(S7’s name),
where’s your book like this?”
S7: (shrugs)
The initiating may also be done by students.
(30) S5: “Are we
here, (teacher’s name)?”
T: “Yeah.”
These initiation-response exchanges are organized
in two forms, somewhat analogous to Philips’ (1972) notion
of participant structures. In the first form, the teacher interacts
with students individually
on a one-at-a-time basis. For example:
(83) T: “You’re
finished, (S5’s name)?”
S5: (nods)
This form of teacher-student interaction occurs
in two domains of performance, constituted by
vertically co-occurring
social
phenomena: (1) private performance,
and (2) public performance. Teacher-student interactions
in
private performance typically are signified by
(1) both teacher
and student lowering their
voices; (2) decreases in the interpersonal distance
between the teacher and the student;
and (3) postural orientation by the teacher that more clearly “focuses” on
the individual student to the exclusion of others. Other
members of the class not involved in the private performance
go about their business, manifesting
behavior that indicates not only that they are not involved
in the interaction, but also that they are not paying attention
to the private performance (even
though the private performance may be audible and visually
accessible to them).
Lines 30-35 of the video-taped transcript
provide an illustration of teacher-student interactions
in private performance.
The teacher is
standing at the head
of Table B (Fig. 1). The teacher, S5 and S7 are speaking
in lower voices than
they use
in public performance.
Figure 1. Teacher and student locations during the lesson.
[click on image for a bigger view]
(30) S5: “Are we here, (teacher’s
name)?”
The teacher orients her upper trunk, head and gaze to S5.
S6 and S7 briefly glance at the teacher and S5, then return
to
looking at their
books.
T: “Yeah.”
S7: “Here, (teacher’s name).”
The teacher shifts her feet, orients her upper trunk, head
and gaze to S7. S5 and S6 glance briefly at the teacher
and return
to their
books.
T: “Yeah, on this side.”
Teacher points. S5 and S6 glance briefly at S7’s
book, then return to their own.
S7: “This one?”
Teacher remains oriented to S7. S5 and S6 continue looking
at their books.
T: “Yeah.”
Teacher-student interactions in public performance
typically are signified by both teacher and student speaking louder,
with greater
interpersonal
distance between the participants, and a more inclusive
postural focus by the teacher.
In public performances, it is not necessary for other members
of the class to “actively disattend” to the
interaction. An example of the teacher interacting with
an individual student on a one-at-a-time basis in public
performance
occurs in lines 83-84 of the transcript. The teacher is
sitting on the end of Table A (Fig. 1); S5 is in his seat.
The teacher’s
voice is noticeably louder than it was in the example cited
above of private performance.
(83) T: “You’re
finished, (S5’s name)?”
S5: (nods affirmatively).
The second form of teacher-student interaction
in the lesson is the teacher interacting with the class as a whole
group:
(47) T: “What page are we on?”
S2: “Ah--”
Many: “40.”
By definition this form occurs only in public
performance.
The teacher interacting with individual students on
a one-at-a-time basis in private performance, the teacher interacting
with individual students
on
a one-at-a-time basis in public performance, and
the teacher interacting with the class as a whole group comprise three
social contexts
for interaction in
the lesson. These three contexts are organized together
in a more superordinate level of social organization.
Initially, the video-tape behavior record of the
lesson was viewed with the sound off. In attending
to nonverbal
channels
of behavior,
the reading
lesson
seems
to be divided into three segments (Figure 2). In
the first, the students move their chairs from around
Table
B to the
locations shown in Figure
1. The teacher
moves around the room, handing out the materials
for the lesson. In the second segment, the teacher
remains
seated
on the end
of Table A. During
the third
segment, the teacher again moves around the room
interacting with the
students.
Figure 2. Segments in the lesson.
[click on image for a bigger view]
Erickson (1975) and Scheflen (1973) demonstrate
that locational and postural shifts can be reliable indicators
of junctures
between behavioral
segments.
The nonverbal behavioral clues that indicate the
junctures of these three segments are: (1) the students’ relocating
and the teacher’s beginning to circulate
around the room; (2) the teacher’s seating
herself on the end of Table A and remaining there
throughout the second segment; (3) the teacher’s
getting up and again circulating around the room;
and (4) the students’ handing
in their books.
Examination of the verbal behavior
of the teacher and students during the entire sequence
provides
further
indication
that the lesson can
be divided
into three distinct social units. In the first segment,
the teacher addresses 87.5
percent of her utterances to individual students
and, on these occasions, interacts with students
in private
performance.
In the second segment
only 25.9 percent
of her utterances are directed to individual students
in private
performance. Another 24.1 percent of her utterances
address individual students
on a one-at-a-time basis, but in public performance.
Fifty percent of her
utterances
are addressed
to the class as a whole group. In the third segment,
89.9 percent of the teacher’s
utterances are addressed to individual students in
private performance. Her only public utterance in
this segment, “Ok, hand in your books,” closes
the lesson. These data are summarized in Table 1.
Further
indication that segment 1 and segment 2 are distinct
contexts for social interaction is provided
by the patterns
of “who initiates” the exchanges.
In the first segment, the teacher initiates only
28.6 percent of the exchanges, while in the second
segment, she initiates 85 percent of the exchanges.
The third
segment does not differ from the second along the “who
initiates” dimension:
the teacher initiates 85.7 percent of the exchanges.
In the second segment, however, 76 percent of the
initiations
are in public performance, compared with 14.3 percent
in the third segment. These data are summarized in
Table 2.
The data summarized in Tables 1 and 2 demonstrate
coherence of particular patterns in the teacher’s
and the students’ verbal
behavior within the junctures suggested by their
non-verbal behavior. Tables 1 and 2 also show the
distinctiveness
of the patterns of verbal behavior in each segment
within the lesson. Figure 2 provides schematic orientation
to
the structural and temporal relationships
of the segments in the lesson.
TABLE 1
Focus of Teacher Utterances in Public and Private
Performance
|
|
|
Public Performance |
|
|
Private Performance |
|
Whole
Class |
Small
Group |
Individual
Student |
Other |
Subtotal
(Public) |
Individual
Student |
Segment 1
Segment 2
Segment 3
Total |
0
27
1
28 |
0
0
0
0
|
0
13
0
13 |
1
0
0
1 |
1
40
1
42 |
7
14
8
29 |
|
Total
Utterances |
% Total
Utterances
Public |
% Total
Utterances
Private |
Segment 1
Segment 2
Segment 3
Total |
8
54
9
71 |
12.5
24.1
11.1
|
87.5
25.9
89.9
|
TABLE 2
Teacher and Students as Initiators in Verbal Exchanges
in Public and Private Contexts
|
|
Public |
|
|
Private |
|
|
|
|
Teacher
Initiates
Exchange |
Student
Initiates
Exchange |
% Public
Initiation |
Teacher
Initiates
Exchange |
Student
Initiates
Exchange |
% Private
Initiation |
% Teacher
Initiates
|
% Student
Initiates |
Segment 1
Segment 2
Segment 3 |
0
30
1 |
0
8
1
|
0
76.0
14.3 |
2
12
5 |
5
0
1 |
100.0
24.0
85.7 |
28.6
84.0
85.7 |
71.4
16.0
14.3 |
The teacher and the students, however,
do not generate this observable interactional order for its own
sake. Nadel (1957, p.158) points out that social organizations “have
jobs to do.” At the level of focusing
on the social organization of the lesson as a classroom event, Nadel’s
observation is relevant. Classroom lessons
are organized around topically oriented tasks and activities. The
particular task or activity is set by the teacher and
constitutes a major element in the “teacher’s
agenda” for the
lesson (Mehan, et al., 1976). Another major
element of the “teacher’s
agenda” for a lesson includes establishing
the broad structural configuration of the
social organization of the lesson in ways
that contribute to the “working
through” of the task or activity by
the students in the allotted time. It is
the teacher who establishes whether the class
will “work through” the
task or activity as a whole group, in small
groups, or as individual students. A third
major element of the teacher’s agenda
for a lesson is that the students “work
through” the task or activity in orderly
ways -- ways that do not disrupt the accomplishment
of the task or otherwise irritate
the teacher. Thus, from a teacher’s
perspective, at the level of the lesson as
a classroom event, the “job” of
the social organization of the lesson is
to facilitate the “working through” of
the task or activity by the students in orderly
ways within the allotted time.
In this lesson,
the task the students work through is a “reading
readiness” exercise
from a page in a workbook. The broad structural
configuration of the social organization
of the lesson is that the students work through
this exercise as a whole group
under the direction of the teacher. The task
and the mode for its working through were
decided on by the teacher before the beginning
of the lesson (personal communication).
That this lesson plan was accomplished by
the students and the teacher in an orderly
way is observable in the behavior record
of the lesson. The social
order evident in the lesson, however, is
not the product of the students following
an explicit script written by the teacher.
The evident order is “assembled” in
the interaction between the teacher and the
students during the course of the lesson.
Social
Organization and Control in Segment 1
During
segment 1, the teacher and the students “get ready” for
the reading readiness exercise. As they get ready,
they conjointly assemble the structural
configuration for working through the readiness
exercise as a whole group. We now examine aspects of the
interactional order evident in segment
1 for the purposes of gaining insight into its accomplishment
by the Koyukon teacher and the students.
Segment
1 occurs between the close of the bilingual lesson and the beginning
of the
working through
of the reading
readiness exercise by the class.
During the bilingual lesson, the students
and the bilingual teacher
are clustered
around Table B (see Figure 3). The instructional
activities completed during the
lesson are (1) playing a game with flash
cards in which the teacher displays a
card and individual
students
take turns
responding
to the picture on
the card; (2) singing, in unison, a song
about birds
in the Koyukon language; and (3) singing,
in unison, “Good Morning Teacher” in
Koyukon and English.
Figure 3. Teacher and student locations during the bilingual lesson.
[click on image for a bigger view]
The structural configuration
of the social organization of the bilingual
lesson
is that of a “whole group activity.” During
each of the instructional activities,
members’ involvement and attention
are directed toward participation in
the efforts of the group as a whole to
accomplish the task. Students participate
verbally in the bilingual lesson as individuals
(the flash card game) and as
a group in unison (singing the songs).
The individual student turns during the
game are embedded in the interactional
organization of “playing the
game” by the class as a group.
This is demonstrated by the following:
-
The allocation of turns among members
of the group provides for participation
by the
students
in ways
that make for
an orderly progression
of the game,
rather than simply the orderly completion
of the turn
- Students not involved
in taking a turn display behavior
making it evident
they
are attending
to the turns of
others and are
thus still
part of the
group playing the game
- The instructional
activity is completed when the game ends, rather
than when
a turn is completed.
The working through
of the reading readiness exercise by the class, the episode which
follows segment
1 in the behavior
record, also
is structured
as a whole
group activity. Students participate
verbally both in individual public performance
and
in unison
as a group.
Individual
student verbal participation
is embedded
in the social organization of the instructional
activity as
a whole group activity. This is indicated
in the following ways:
- Individual student
verbal participation is keyed to the rate the class as a group
works through
the exercise.
It
is inappropriate
for students
to attend
verbally, in public performance,
to parts of the exercise ahead of
or behind those being focused on
by the group
- The students not “doing” an individual
public performance during the working through of the exercise display
behavior that indicates they
are “paying attention” to
the person who has the floor
-
The completion of an individual
verbal “turn” in
public performance does not complete
the instructional
activity or absolve the participant
from
manifesting behavioral signs
of on-going involvement in the
groups’ efforts.
The instructional activity is
completed when the group has
worked through the
exercise to the teacher’s satisfaction.
Segment
1, in occurring between the bilingual
lesson and the reading readiness
exercise,
is not only
temporally intermediate between
them, but it is structurally
intermediate as well. It is the occasion
for the restructuring of the class
for the reading
readiness
exercise as
a whole
group activity
after the
dissolution of the whole group structure
of the bilingual lesson.
The social
organizational structure of the bilingual lesson begins to dissolve
toward
the end of the
last instructional
activity
in the lesson,
the singing
in unison of “Good Morning
Teacher,” when
the bilingual teacher begins putting
materials from directly
in front of her on Table B into a
cloth bag. After she begins doing
this, but
before the song is completed, S5
slides her chair
away from
Table B.
With the ending of the song,
the interactional organization of
the
bilingual lesson
more evidently dissolves.
S2 and S3 slide
their
chairs back from
Table B and posturally orient themselves
to each other. S5 stands on his chair
and fidgets. As the bilingual teacher
says, “OK” (line 1),
S1 slides back his chair and stands
up; S6 moves his chair back, stretches,
and looks at
the books in the bookcase along the
wall behind him. As S7 asks the bilingual
teacher, “We’ll play
that cards tomorrow?” (line
2), S4 slides her chair back from
Table B, stands up and fidgets. S5
stands and
stretches
as
the bilingual teacher responds to
the question (lines 5-6).
Coincident
with line 7 of the transcript, S1
and S3 begin moving their chairs
to the locations
indicated
in Figure
1. S4 begins
relocating after commenting, “Oh,
boy” (line 7), on the bilingual
teacher’s response to S7’s
question. S3 also comments on this
response from halfway across the
room (line 8). S2 stands and begins
moving his chair after notifying
the class that “Time’s
up” (line 9). While the students
continue relocating, the bilingual
teacher leaves
the room.
The dissolution of the organizational
structure of the bilingual lesson
by the teacher
and the students
partially
accomplishes
the transition between the bilingual
lesson and the readiness exercise.
The general structure of interaction
during
the bilingual
lesson and the
working through
of the reading
readiness exercise is organized around
the accomplishment of instructional
activities. The general structure
of interactions
during the
transition accomplished in segment
1 is organized
around getting
ready for an instructional
activity.
The video-tape behavior
record shows the teacher and the students “getting
ready” for the reading exercise
by completing the following tasks:
-
The students relocate from their
positions during the bilingual
lesson to the positions
indicated
in Figure 1
- The workbooks
are distributed to the students
by the teacher
- The students
find the page in the workbook containing
the
readiness
exercise.
The ways the students
and the teacher mutually go about completing these
tasks also establish
the social
organizational
framework
for working
through the reading readiness exercise
as a whole group activity.
As S1,
S2, s3, and S4 are moving their chairs after the close of
the bilingual
lesson,
the certificated
teacher
clears
her desk
and gathers
up the workbooks
to be passed out. When the students
begin to arrive at their destinations,
the
teacher stands,
goes
to Table
D, and starts
to lay a workbook
at S3’s position
(see Figure 4). S3 and S4 are
involved in a “traffic
jam” at
the lower end of Table D and
are not yet in place. S2 has
reached
his destination,
but he is not yet sitting down.
The teacher puts S3’s
workbook back in the pile she
is carrying, turns, and goes
to look
out the window
between the
bookcase and the piles of materials
shown at the top of Figure 4.
She remains looking
out
the window
until S2,
S3, and S4 are seated in position.
Figure 4. Teacher movements around the room during segment 1.
[click on image for a bigger view]
As
they settle in, she turns from
the window, moves to Table
D, and
hands
S2 and S4 their
workbooks.
While this
is happening,
S3
begins
to dig
in her desk. The teacher does
not hand S3 her workbook, but
goes
to Table
A and hands
S1 his. She then moves back toward
Table D, but pauses about halfway
there. S3 is still looking in
her desk. The
teacher turns, goes to Table
B, and gives
S5
and S6 their workbooks.
S7’s
workbook is missing from the
stack being passed out
by the teacher.
It is not
until this point
in the segment
that the teacher speaks:
(21)
T: “(S7’s name), where’s
your book like this?”
S7: (shrugs)
T: “Look in your desk.”
(25) S7: “Do you remember Miss____took it.”
(27) T: “That was (S3’s name)’s.”
The teacher
then turns, goes to Table D and hands S3 her
workbook. Moving
from Table
D to the piles
of materials
indicated in
the upper left of
Figure 3, she looks for S7’s
workbook. On finding it, she
exclaims, “Oh,
here it is!” (line 29)
and returns to Table B and
hands S7 her workbook.
S5 asks, “Are
we here, (teacher’s name)?” (line
30). The teacher responds, “Yeah” (line
31). S7, who by this time has
opened her workbook, points
to the proper page and asks, “Here,
(teacher’s name)?” (line
33). The teacher, leaning over
and pointing at the correct
page in S7’s
book, replies, “Yeah,
on this side” (line 34).
S7, seemingly still not sure
of herself, again points to
the page and asks, “This
one?” (line
34). The teacher responds, “Yeah” (line
35).
Turning from Table B, the
teacher moves to Table A. As
she does
so, S1 turns
around with a puzzled
look
on his
face.
The teacher,
arriving
at
S1’s location,
points to the correct page
in the workbook lying open
in front of S1 and comments, “Right
there” (line 40).
She
then moves to the end of Table
A and sits on top of
it, facing
the class.
By
the time
the teacher
is
seated, all
the students
have their
workbooks open to the proper
page. All the students, with
the exception of S2, are
also either
looking up at the teacher or
looking at their books, ready
to begin
the exercise as a whole group.
The structural
transition
between the bilingual lesson
and the readiness
exercise
has been
completed.
The most evident
aspect of the teacher’s behavior during the accomplishment
of the transition is her
movement around the room (see Figure 4). One of the functions of
this movement
is the exercise of control over the behavior of the
students (Erickson and Mohatt,
1976). This exercising of control through “pounding
the classroom beat” is
more the product of the teacher
synchronizing her movement
around the room with the
students’ readiness
to receive the workbooks
and to begin focusing on
the material
than it is deterrence effected
by the immediate presence
of the “fuzz.” The
teacher does not begin moving
around the room when the
students begin relocating;
she also
remains seated
when the bilingual teacher
leaves the room. She allows
the students to proceed to
their new locations without
exercising “teacher
stares” or
other overt nonverbal control
devices. The teacher also
abstains from exerting
verbal
control.
It is not until the
students have essentially
arrived
at their destinations
that
the teacher stands up
and begins
moving around
the classroom.
When she arrives
at Table D, S2 is not yet
sitting down; S3 and S4 are
still positioning
their
chairs and also
are not
seated.
Rather
than invoking verbal
or nonverbal sanctions or
prompts to “hurry them
up,” the
teacher moves to the window
and looks out until S2, S3,
and S4
are ready to
begin focusing
on the material.
As she hands
S2 and S4 their books, S3
begins looking
in her desk.
She is not
yet ready.
Once again,
the teacher neither invokes
sanctions nor
overtly
directs
S3’s attention to the
workbook. She proceeds to
Table A and hands S1 his
workbook. Leaving Table A,
the teacher pauses and checks
on S3. She is still
not ready, so the teacher
moves without comment to
Table B. When she leaves
Table B, on the way to look
for S7’s
workbook, the teacher again
checks on S3. S3 is no
longer looking
for something
in her desk and is now ready.
The teacher then hands S3
her workbook.
Examination
of the teacher’s verbal
behavior during segment 1
furthers the impression that
the teacher is acting
in concert with the students,
rather
than overtly controlling
them. Seven of the eight
teacher utterances in the
segment are directed to individuals
and occur in private performance.
The teacher and
student are at an intimate
distance from each other
and the rest of the students
do not serve as an audience
to the interaction. The teacher’s
verbal behavior is thus coupled
to her movement around the
room; she talks with
the students
who are in closest proximity.
Just
as she does in her movements
around the room, the teacher
seems to be synchronizing
her verbal
behavior with the “rhythms” of
the class. The interactional
device she employs is that
of allowing students to be
the primary initiators in
successful verbal exchanges.
She initiates only two of
the seven successful
verbal exchanges in which
she is involved during segment
1. Both of these instances
concern the whereabouts of
S7’s
workbook and are tangential
to the accomplishment of
the transition.
This portrayal
of the Koyukon teacher’s
exercise of her classroom
authority is consistent with
Erickson and Mohatt’s
description of the exercise
of authority by the Indian
teacher
in a classroom on the Canadian
reserve.
Social control is
distributed in Classroom
1 as a shared
quantity -- leadership
by teacher and
by students
interpenetrates
rather
than being
divided into
separate compartments.
The teacher clearly has “control” of
the students, but achieves
this partly by paying much
attention
to the rhythms
of student
activity,
and judging when students
are ready for things to
change (Erickson and Mohatt,
1976,
p.
15).
The absence of the exercise
of overt control by the teacher
over the behavior
of the
students, and the
distribution
of social
control as
a “shared quantity” during
the segment, directs attention
to the accomplishment of
the transition as a
mutual achievement by the teacher and
the students.
The establishment
of the whole group structure
which
completes
the transition
is a mutual
assembly by
the teacher and
the students as
they engage in “interactional
work” during segment
1. “Interactional work” is
used here to mean the ways
the teacher and the students,
in negotiating the definition
of
the situation, “inform” each
other of “what is going
on.” This
interactional work is “framed” by
the classroom routines established
over the course of
the year. The students
know that at the end of the
bilingual lesson, S1, S2,
S3, and S4 will
move their chairs to the
locations shown in
figure 1,
as they have
been doing for
nearly the entire school
year. The students also know
that Reading
typically follows
the Bilingual
class in the daily schedule.
One
aspect of the “interactional
work” going on between
the teacher and the students
is the confirmation that
students are to proceed according
to “normal
form.” The teacher,
by not indicating that the
students are to do otherwise, “informs” the
students that S1, S2, S3,
and S4 are to move their
chairs back to their seats
as they do each day. The
students thus
proceed according to a “business
as usual” pattern
without requiring direction
from the teacher on who is
to move,
where, or
how.
For the students, there
is some ambiguity inherent
in
the normal
form of the
social organizational
structure to be
used on any
given day
to accomplish
Reading.
Sometimes Reading is done
by students on an individual
basis,
sometimes
it is done
in small
groups, and
sometimes it is
done by the class
as a whole
group (teacher personal communication).
There is ambiguity, too,
for the students in
the normal form of the instructional
activity for Reading on a
particular day: it may
be
working
in a workbook,
playing games, listening
to a story, or doing
seatwork made up by the teacher.
A
second aspect of the interactional
work going on in the accomplishment
of the transition
is the reduction
of ambiguity
when the normal
forms contain an array
of options. The ambiguity
inherent in the normal
form of the instructional
activity for Reading is reduced
when
the teacher
informs the students
of the
coming instructional
activity by passing out the
workbooks. Evidence of the
workbooks also indicates
that
Reading comes next, confirming
that the
daily schedule
is proceeding
as it typically does.
The
students still do not know which page in the workbook
they will be
working on.
Typically,
when
the students
have worked
in this particular
workbook,
they have taken each page
in
sequence. Occasionally,
they work on pages
out of sequence.
All the students open their
workbooks to page 40, the
page following
the last page
worked
on. S1,
S5, and
S7 are still
uncertain that
page 40 is
the correct
page. S5 and S7, individually,
in private performance,
verbally elicit
confirmation
from the teacher.
S1 elicits confirmation
from the teacher
by turning and
looking at her as she leaves
Table B and begins heading
in his direction.
The teacher,
without a word from S1,
continues to his seat
and confirms he has the
correct page.
There is still ambiguity
as to whether Reading will
be
conducted
as individual
work, small
group work,
or work
done by the
class as a whole
group. The
first bit of information
the students process to
reduce this ambiguity
is that, generally, pages
in
the particular
workbook
being handed out by the
teacher are worked through
as a whole
group. Secondly, if
the work is to be done
on an individual basis,
the teacher typically indicates
this
in
private performance as
she
hands out material. If
the instructional activity
is to be
completed
as a small
group,
the teacher typically begins
convening these groups
as she moves around
the room. The
fact that she
neither establishes that
the work be done
on an individual
basis, nor indicates the
formation of small groups,
informs
the students that Reading
will be conducted as a
whole group
activity.
The accomplishment
of the transition by the
class
without explicit
directions or
the
exercise of
overt control
by the teacher
suggests a high degree
of congruence between the
assumptions and knowledge
held by the teacher
and the students
about the appropriate conduct
of everyday social life
in classrooms. For a group
of students and a teacher
to be
able to
leave
many things unstated as
they
accomplish a classroom
transition
which includes
a change in
teacher, the relocation
of more than half the class,
the distribution of
material, and the orientation
of the students to that
material, requires that
the teacher
and students continually
arrive at mutually consistent
interpretations of what
is going on and share mutually
consistent understandings
and expectations for how
to proceed.
Not only are
many things left unstated
in accomplishing
the
transition,
but the interactional
work
between the teacher
and the students
proceeds smoothly.
Segment
1 takes some l31 seconds
from juncture to juncture.
Thirty-seven
seconds
of this time
were taken
up in locating S7’s
workbook, leaving about
94 seconds for the completion
of the transition.
That
a group
of kindergarten
students and
a teacher complete the
tasks they do and get ready
for Reading
in such a short
time is an impressive indicator
in itself of the smoothness
of the interactional
work
going
on between
the teacher
and the students.
A second
indicator of interactional
smoothness in segment 1
is the relative infrequency
of “repair work, “ the
channeling or correction
of divergent student behavior
by the teacher.
Equally important
is that the repair
work is done smoothly
by the teacher without
issuing explicit directions
or otherwise
overtly exercising
control over
the students. The repair
work done by the teacher
during segment 1 is accomplished
by
not replying to
particular verbal initiations
by students.
Two instances
of repair work by the teacher
involve
the
non-Indian student
in the
class, S2. While
the teacher
is involved in
verbal exchanges in
private performance
with S7 (concerning the
whereabouts of S7’s
workbook; lines 21-27),
S2 turns in his seat, looks
directly at the teacher
and says, in a loud voice, “OK,
ah, we’re on page
40” (line
24).
The teacher does not
respond or acknowledge
S2’s contribution.
S2 fidgets a bit and, looking
directly at the teacher,
calls out, “We’re
on page 40.” Again,
the teacher does not acknowledge
S2. S2 fidgets
a bit
more and
continues looking
at the teacher, seemingly
waiting for acknowledgement.
He then
turns back in
his seat and looks
at his workbook.
By not responding to S2,
the teacher channels his
behavior
away from calling out and
interrupting an ongoing
exchange in private
performance
without overt direction
or control.
The process
of getting ready is accomplished
without
the
exercise of overt control
over the behavior
of the students
by the teacher
with
one exception.
As the teacher
begins to sit on the end
of Table A facing the class,
all
the students,
with the
exception of
S2, are quiet
and are
either
looking at page
40 or up at
the teacher. S2 continues
to talk
to
S4 (who is looking up at
the teacher, not
attending to S2). The teacher
marks the juncture between
segment 1
and segment
2 by asking, “OK,
what page are we on?” (line
42). The students, with
the exception of S2, respond
in chorus, “Forty” (line
44). S2 continues talking,
addressing his talk to
S4, who is attending to
the teacher (lines 41 and
45). The teacher
orients towards S2 and
in a noticeably louder
voice says, “What
page are we on?” (line
47). S2 turns toward the
teacher, stops talking
toward s4, and says, “Ah” (line
48). The rest of the class
responds in chorus, “Forty” (line
49). S2 then responds, “Forty” (line
50), having been brought
into the fold.
The teacher,
in orienting her posture
and gaze to
S2, raising
her voice
and asking
the question
again,
escalates
to a
more overt
exercise of
her control
in informing
S2 of what is going on.
It is worth noting that
this
escalation to
a more overt
form of control
by the
teacher is yet somewhat
oblique.
The
teacher
does not
spotlight S2 in verbal
rebuke or sanction; rather
she informs
him
by orienting
to him, raising her
voice
and repeating
to the class
as a
whole a question
that has already been answered.
The
Koyukon teacher’s
exercise of her classroom
authority is consistent
with what might be expected
for an Indian teacher,
generalizing from Philips
(1972). First, the students
are allowed a great deal
of self-determination in
their actions as they “get
ready” for Reading.
The teacher does not attempt
to exercise continual and
pervasive control over
the students’ behavior.
Second, the teacher does
not “single out” in
public performance either
divergent student behavior
or student uncertainty
about
what is going on;
rather, she uses a variety
of channels for communication
to
inform
the students of what is
going on.
The Koyukon teacher’s
exercise of her classroom
authority is also consistent
with Erickson and Mohatt’s
(1976) findings and interpretations
of the exercise of authority
by the Indian teacher on
the Northern Ontario Reserve.
First,
the Koyukon teacher typically
exercises her authority
in covert ways during the
segment. Social control
is distributed as a shared
quantity as the teacher
and the students “get
ready” for Reading.
Second, there is a high
degree of interactional “smoothness” in
the ways the teacher and
the students do their interactional
work. Third, the teacher
does not put individual
students
in the “classroom
spotlight.”
Conclusions
This example suggests that
a more comprehensive
study of how’ Athabaskan
teachers (who are few
in number) and how non-Indian teachers exercise
their
classroom
authority
might provide illuminating
insights into the workings
of Alaskan cross-cultural classrooms.
The consistency
of the example with the
findings and interpretations of
Philips (1972)
and Erickson and Mohatt
(1976) suggests that systematic inquiry
into discontinuities
between
the way social life is
conducted in Alaskan Native
communities
and in Alaskan classrooms
may provide useful information for
developing and
effecting
styles of teaching and
classroom
control more appropriate
to Alaskan Native students.
The
analysis presented in this paper is a limited
example
of a Koyukon
accomplishment of a classroom
social organization.
The
analysis assumes
the point of view
that the ways the teacher
and
the Athabaskan students
conduct themselves
during
the segment are culturally organized. Within
the context of this paper,
this assumption remains
unexamined.
Erickson and Mohatt,
however,
argue that
such an assumption
can be empirically examined
and tested.
We will argue
that a shared sense of pacing
between
teacher and
students (part of their
mutually congruent
interactional
competence
-- their
shared culture as
defined by Goodenough)
is manifested behaviorally
in
an interactional
smoothness
where presence
or absence is
empirically
observable.
This observable smoothness,
in getting through
an event and in getting
from one
event to
the next,
can be taken
as an indicator
of
shared expectations
and interpretive
strategies on the
part of the participants
in the interactional
scene (1976,
p.
16).
While this paper
has displayed the smoothness
in the conduct
of the
interactional work
between the
teacher
and the students,
it
does
not argue that this
smoothness is the product
of the “Athabaskaness” of
the participants. To
be able to do so requires
that lack of smoothness
in the non-Indian student’s
interactional work
be demonstrated. This
is indeed the case
in behavior during
the segment. At the
end of the bilingual
lesson, S2 does not
begin relocating
until he announces, “Time’s
up” (line 9),
although the other
students are already
moving their chairs.
S2 tries to interrupt
an ongoing verbal
exchange, in private
performance, to let
the teacher know, in
public performance,
that he knows the correct
page. It can be argued
that while a student’s
seeking the classroom
spotlight is inappropriate
social behavior in
an Indian classroom,
it would be considered
reasonable behavior
for a non-Indian student
in a non-Indian classroom.
However, interrupting
an ongoing teacher-student
exchange
would probably also
be deemed inappropriate
in a non-Indian classroom.
Finally, S2 is not “ready” when
the instructional activity
begins. These
demonstrations of
interactional incompetence
by the non-Indian student
are too few and too
varied to allow
convincing demonstration,
but they are suggestive.
They may well be
the product
of differences
in the cultural organization
of behavior between
the non-Indian student
and the
Indian members of the
class.
Elaboration of
the analysis to include
the entire
lesson would
also provide
an example
to contrast
with one
of Philips’ findings
in classrooms with
non-Indian teachers.
The prevailing participant
structure during segment
2 of the lesson
is the first participant
structure described
in Philips, i.e., the
teacher interacts with
all of the students.
In Warm Springs classrooms,
the Indian students
participated
infrequently in classroom
events organized in
this way. In this lesson
conducted by an Indian
teacher, the Indian
students participate
freely and frequently.
Microethnographic analysis
of how this is accomplished
by the teacher and
the
students could provide
some illuminating insights
into “the silent
Indian student” syndrome.
References
Bolm, J. and J. Gumperz. “Social Meaning
in Linguistic Structure: Code-Switching in Norway,” in J. Gumperz
and D. Humes, eds. Directions in Socio-linguistics. New York: Holt,
Rinehart and Winston, 1972.
Cazden, C. “The
Neglected Situation
in Child Language Research,” in
D. Williams, ed.
Language and Poverty. Chicago: University
of Chicago Press,
1970.
Cicourel, A. Cognitive
Sociology. New
York: The Free
Press,
1974.Cicourel,
A.; K. Jennings;
S.
Jennings;
K. Leiter;
R. Mackay; H. Mehan,
and P. Roth. Language
Use and School
Performance. New
York: Academic
Press, Inc.,
1974.
Dunkin, M. J. and
B. J. Biddle. The
Study
of Teaching.
New
York: Holt,
Rinehart
and Winston,
Inc., 1974.
Eddy, E. M. Becoming
a Teacher. New
York: Teachers
College
Press, 1969.
Erickson, F. “One Function of Proxemic Shifts in Face-to-Face Interaction,” in
A. Kendon; M. R. Key, and R.
Harris, eds. The
Organization of
Behavior in
Face-to-Face
Interaction,
1975.
Erickson, F. and
G. Mohatt. “The Social Organization of Participation
Structures in Two Classrooms of Indian Students.” Paper
presented at the Annual Meeting
of the American Educational Research
Association,
San Francisco,
1976.
Firth, R. “Social Organization and Social Change.” Journal
of the Royal Anthropological
Institute of Great
Britain and Ireland. Vol. 84,
1954.
Flanders, N. A.
Analyzing Teaching
Behavior.
Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley,
1970.Florio,
S. “Issues in the Analysis of the Structure of Classroom Interaction.” Qualifying
paper, Harvard Graduate School
of Education, 1976.
Garfinkel, H. Studies
in Ethnomethodology.
New Jersey:
Prentice-Hall,
Inc., 1967.
Goffman, E. Encounters
- Two Studies in
the Sociology of
Interaction. New
York: The Bobbs-Merrill
Co.,
Inc., 1961.
Goffman, E. “The Neglected Situation,” in
J. J. Gumperz and D. Hymes, eds.
The Ethnography
of Communication.
American Anthropologist, 66,
1964.
Good, T. L. and
J. E. Brophy. Looking
in Classrooms.
New York: Harper
and Row, 1973.
Goodenough, W. “Cultural Anthropology and Linguistics,” in
D. Hymes, ed. Language
in Culture and
Society. New York: Harper
and Row, 1964.
Goodenough, W.
Culture, Language,
and Society.
Reading, MA:
Addison-Wesley,
1971.
Goodenough, W. “Multiculturalism as the Normal Human Experience.” Paper
presented at the Annual Meeting
of the American Anthropological
Association,
San Francisco,
1975.
Goodenough, W. “Rethinking ‘Status’ and ‘Role’:
Toward a General Model of the Cultural Organization of Social Relationships,” in
M. Bantone, ed. The
Relevance of Models
for Social Anthropology.
London: Tavistock,
1965.
Grambs, J. “The Roles of the Teacher,” in
J. Chilcott, N.
Greenberg, and H. Wilson, eds. Readings in
the Socio-Cultural
Foundations of Education. California: Wadsworth,
1968.
Grant, B. M. and
D. G. Hennings.
The Teacher
Moves: An Analysis
of Non-Verbal
Activity.
New York: Teachers
College
Press,
1971.
Jackson, P. W.
Life in Classrooms.
New
York:
Holt, Rinehart,
and Winston, 1968.
Kendon, A. “Introduction,” in
A. Kendon, R. Harris and M. R.
Key, ads. Organization
of Behavior
in
Face-to-Face Interaction. The
Hague: Mouton, 1975.
Mehan, H.; C. Cazden;
L. Coles; S. Fisher
and N.
Maroules. The
Social Organization
of Classroom
Lessons. La Jolla,
California: University
of California,
Center for Information
Processing,
San Diego, 1976.
Nadel, S. The Theory
of Social Structure.
Glencoe,
III:
Free Press, 1957.
Philips, S. “Participant Structures and Communicative Competence:
Warm Springs Children in Community and Classroom,” in
C. Cazden; V. John, and D. Hymes,
eds. Functions
of Language in
the Classroom.
New York: Teachers
College Press, 1972.
Scheflen, A. E.
Communicational
Structure Analysis
of a Psychotherapy
Transaction.
Bloomington:
Indiana University
Press, 1973.
Spradley, J. Culture
and Cognition:
Rules, Maps
and Plans. San
Francisco: Chandler, 1972.
Waller, W.
The Sociology
of Teaching.
New York:
John Wiley
and Sons,
Inc., 1932.
|