Alaskan Eskimo Education:
A Film Analysis of Cultural
Confrontation in the Schools
3/Methodology of filming
and analysis
WHY USE FILM?
Bethel is a hub of communication and
transportation for planes that fly in by the hundreds from all points of
the compass. Bush planes on skis roar
off the river ice for the multitude of Eskimo villages-down on the Bering
Sea, Out on the vast tundra, and up and down the Kuskokwim and its winding
tributaries. By summer the river is a highway for barges and outboard-driven
fishing skiffs, by winter a frozen path for dog sleds, and increasingly
the gasoline-driven snowmobile. All journeys begin and end on the frozen
river front before the Northern Commercial Company post.
On my first day
in Bethel I stood on the river piling and looked down on the snarling or
sleeping sled dogs and now silent metallic Sno-Go’s and tried
to picture where these sleds and machines would be at their journey’s
end. In a very few days I too would be flying up river to begin filming
Eskimo education. I would be working alone trying to record, often in
one visit
to an isolated tundra school, sufficient data to define the culture of
each school and each class.
In two weeks the field team, John Connelly,
Ray Barnhardt, his wife Carol, and their baby son John, would arrive
for a two months residence. John,
Ray, and Carol would meticulously study first, fifth, eight, ninth
and tenth grades
in the Bethel Consolidated School. They would execute “Draw-a-Man” tests
and questionnaires and schedule interviewing with teachers, students,
parents, and important village persons.
Six months earlier I had sat
in Boulder, Colorado, with the leaders and regional fleldworkers
of the National Study of American Indian
Education.
Each day
for two weeks we gathered in seminars redesigning this verbal methodology,
testing
the questionnaires on each other and on Indians in the local Public
Health hospital. What would be the returns of my colleagues using
these most
precise verbal recordings? What would my film data offer that this
thoroughly programmed study would not gather?
First, we look at the
returns of our colleagues, who make their statements mostly from the study
of questionnaires and scheduled interviews.
This brings in data on one level, and final interpretations
rest on highly
abstracted evidence, the speculative verbal stare-speculative,
because words are abstract
mind signals that are recovered within a wide range of meaning,
first within the Native’s response and later in a researcher’s
interpretation. The data must be seen always over a communication
chasm of words.
Film documentation is nonverbal, hence
at many points less speculative and more open to critical judgment
by a team of analysts. It is,
in this sense,
a very low level of documentary abstraction that is as sensual
as it is intellectual, and therefore offers the analyst emotional-psychological
opportunities
difficult to research from verbal projections.
But why not still-photographic
recording, which would be both cheaper and less cumbersome within the field
circumstance?
Fifteen years of experiment have fixed reasonable
limits of still-photographic evidence. The still camera does a fine job of
inventorying
what
is there. It counts and measures responsibly. But it has serious
limitations
when
motion is of a major research importance. True, one can make
voluminous time-and-motion
scheduled, still-photographic studies, but both making and
analyzing these studies become extremely awkward and more time consuming
than the film
record. Time-and-motion records of the still camera are fixed
points of human phenomena.
Projectively the mind must link behavior together
between these fixed points. The result tends to be a gross
assumption or
to be impressionistic
in judgment.
Film flows photographically and links all
time stations without impressionism, and therefore allows for
authentic observation
of motion.
My colleagues in the National Study were getting
the fixed facts. We hoped film would offer the emotional flow between facts
and allow us
to discuss
genuinely the emotional-psychological behavior of the child
in education. The decision
to research with film was based on advanced studies by anthropologists
and psychologists concerned with nonverbal behavior. We thank
pioneers in
visual anthropology for much of our methodology. Edward T.
Hall formally introduced us to the cultural opportunities of film
analysis. He
demonstrated vividly on film with slow-motion projection
of
three culturally different
groups of people at a fiesta- Indians, Spanish-Americans,
and Anglos-how each moved
consistently within a program of behavior which was totally
synchronized within each group. Could we make a similar record in classrooms
and thereby describe
school culture from nonverbal behavior?
Paul Byers of Columbia
has also carried nonverbal recordings into analysis of group behavior. Byers
has demonstrated that
people
in any cultural
circumstance are consistently programmed in their use of
space and body expression (Mead,
Byers, 1967).
We have used Hall’s concept of proxemics,
space in culture, and we have been constantly grateful
to Ray Birdwhistell (1969) for his studies in kinesics,
the significance of body posture. Birdwhistell analyzed
two minutes of film
made by Gregory Bateson of a disturbed mother and child,
and through kinesics was able to diagnose accurately a
state of psychiatric stress. Film appears
able to document emotional stress and well-being, and this
opportunity has been a major objective in our research.
Paul
Ekman, in association with Langley Porter Clinic in San Francisco, has also
carried our many years of study
in psychiatric
nonverbal
behavior and
stabilized film reading to a point where Langley Porter
can use film responsibly in diagnosis.
Finally, as is
inevitable in visual anthropology, we are indebted to Margaret Mead and Gregory
Bateson for
laying
the cornerstones
that
have made the
studies of nonverbal behavior a reality in anthropology.
Business Character: A Photographic
Analysis (1842) offered many clues to making and analyzing
our film research.
WHAT WE WERE LOOKING FOR
Our study
was focused on
schools,
wherever Eskimo
children received an education. In the problem
of educating Native children, were
particular school systems experiencing more difficulties
and negative results than others?
We were particularly interested in BIA schools
because historically they have set the style of Indian education.
However, the
quality of BIA schools
may
be considerably less crucial an issue than it used
to be, simply because more Indian and Eskimo students are
attending
state
and public schools
than federal
schools today, and the field of Native education
has thereby left the remote reservation for the congested
cities.
Beyond the question of the character of
Native schools, the film study was attempting to chart
the human
and educational behavior
of Eskimo
children on three curves: an ecological-geographic
curve, a cultural-ethnic distribution
curve, and an age cycle curve.
Ranging from the
remote Eskimo village, to the larger more progressive village, to the tundra
city salmon
fishing center, and finally
to the modern White
city of Anchorage, our film sample allowed
the camera to
watch the relative learning
pace of the children as it changed, from the
most undisturbed environment to the most impersonal,
un-Eskimo environment
available in Alaska.
Does school setting affect learning and emotional
well-being? Are teachers less or more
responsive to students’ welfare as Eskimos
in tiny village schools as compared to the
city school environment in Anchorage? Are changes
of concern
related to how teachers teach and how students
learn? Is there real value in making sacrifices
to teach in remote villages? Or is it educationally
more
successful to move Eskimo children into large
centers with greater interaction and fuller
educational
advantages? Is environmental and cultural security
really significant in Native education?
The
second curve followed the range of Eskimo children’s
response to the ethnic composition of the classroom,
from the totally Eskimo schools in
the villages to the predominantly Eskimo student
body in Bethel where 85 percent of the school
children are Eskimo, and finally to the predominantly
White schools
in Anchorage, where only 7 percent of the students
are Native-Indians, Eskimos, and Aleuts. Is
there a relationship between well-being and
the ethnic composition
of the classroom? Do Eskimos respond better
within their own group? How do they respond
as a small minority in the genuinely desegregated
school? This
insight might he very important in discussing
the value of BIA education versus public school
attendance. Should Natives near White communities
be bussed our
of their village schools into the stimulation
of White schools? Or is the concept of the
village ethnic community school one key to
effective education for Native
children? Is there a relationship to the school
dropout rate in this ethnic distribution curve?
The
third curve was the age cycle. The class sample
of the National Study included first,
second, fifth,
eighth,
and
tenth grades.
To correlate with their work,
I covered the same sample of classes, but broadened
this coverage to include early childhood education
as well-Head
Start, kindergarten,
and
pre-first-and
special education classes, all of which we
felt were very important in considering Indian
education.
Thus
the age
curve ranged
from Head Start
to tenth grade,
or from about 4 to 16 or 17 years old. Where
on this curve do Eskimo children exhibit the
most
intensity
in learning?
Is there
an age
or
grade at which
children begin to show signs of alienation
or stress? If the students exhibit stress
at adolescence, is this essentially physiological?
Or is this tress also environmentally related?
In the background
of this
query is,
of course,
the historical
debate over ethnically segregated schools versus
integrated schools.
Questions were also asked
as to the value of Natively relevant material
in the curriculum,
the value
of bilingualism, and the possible educational
empathy
of Native teachers versus Caucasian teachers.
Since the film sample included a few instances
of these
elements, I have
also evaluated,
on a very small
sample, the effect of relevant curriculum and
the resourcefulness of Native teachers in terms
of
relevance and communication.
The filming was
designed to gather samples systematically, class to class and school to
school, so that
later we could search
for the same
behavior
variables
and produce findings that would represent
the impact and significance of twenty hours of
classroom experience.
The
combined evidence
will support observations
of the goals of Eskimo education and the
character of educational programming for Eskimo children.
TECHNICAL
CONSIDERATIONS
How
to make this film record?
Should I use conventional 16mm film? Or should
I use Super 8mm film, which
is considered
an amateur medium? This question was answered
immediately by the budget. To achieve relevance
would require
many hours of
film.
Sixteen-millimeter would
skyrocket the cost of the research. The Super
8 image is satisfactorily sharp. The cost
of a Super
8 camera
that
would allow us to
make time lapse studies
was relatively low. So was the cost of a
projector designed
to study time lapse frame for frame (the
Eastman Ektomaric Super
8 projector).
Also,
processing costs thousands of dollars in
16 mm. In many ways it was a hard photographic
decision to make, for almost surely it would
limit our film to research only; to date
it is
extremely
difficult
to produce
audience
film
with
Super 8.
But audience use of film was no issue in
this study as the intimacy of these recordings would
ethically
ban
public distribution.
The next decision was
how much to film. Considering the breadth
of our sample we budgeted three
rolls per class,
two rolls
at the conventional
eighteen
frames per second and one roll at automatic
time lapse at two frames per second. This
offered two varieties of recovery. The first
was about nine minutes of
film time devoted to long sequences following
both general and specific activities,
usually shot within a half hour. The second
was a systematic exaggeration of body motion
that
made behavior patterns
easy to see. Two frames
per second, projected at four or six frames
per second,
literally dissects body expression.
Time lapse also allows for rapid frame-for-frame
analysis for refined measure of kinesic,
proxemic, and time
factors.
Ideally, I chose a location where
all of the students and teachers were in view. The
camera
was usually
on a tripod,
and before
filming there
was a
time period allowed for teacher and students
to adjust to the presence of a camera.
Usually halfway through the filming the
camera could be removed from the tripod and I could
wander about
the room
when relevant
material
was not
clearly seen
from the initial position. The camera had
a zoom lens so that individual behavior
could be filmed
intimately
without
moving
the camera.
Slow pans were used to
keep in track all behavior in the class.
After
the filming, an inventory of classrooms was made with the 35mm still camera.
Still
records can document
detail
of walls
and teaching
materials
with more precision than the moving picture
camera and offer an easier analysis opportunity
than
film.
The final decision of technique
was in relation to sound recording. Lip synchronization
was
technically, as well
as financially,
out of the question.
It would have
required many thousands of dollars’ worth
of equipment and the constant presence
of a sound technician. But sound recorded
simultaneously for content
was thoroughly practical. Therefore every
class filmed was sound logged with a
SONY tape recorder. This was the only
documentary way that behavior and curriculum
could be correlated. Also, the noise
level could be related to teaching method.
The recorder was left in a central position
in the class and allowed to run
through the entire time involved in the
filming.
READING THE EDUCATIONAL EVIDENCE
ON FILM
How were we to analyze twenty
hours of film? Frame-for-frame study
on the order of Birdwhistell’s
and Ekman’s work could take four
years. Instead we had to design a study
of controlled judgments by a team of
trained
observers. Slow motion and frame-for-frame
relationships could be afforded only
at key points of significance.
Members
of the team had all studied or were
studying visual anthropology
and
shared a
background of
crosscultural understanding.
Alyce
Cathey, a Eurasian with several years
teaching experience, was currently
teaching English as
a
second language to Chinese children
in San Francisco. Paul Michaels, a
master’s
candidate in education, had taught
several years in Head Start, including
one year in northwestern Alaska. Mack
Ford was a master’s candidate
in anthropology with research experience
in education and photography. Malcolm
Collier was an anthropology major with
years of life among indigenous people
and experience as a photographer.
To
synchronize our analyses we found it
essential to state explicitly how
education
could be
seen on film.
Preliminary
study indicated
that communication is
vividly apparent in the classes that
the judges
felt were educationally alive. Minus
and plus
qualities in
communication appeared
the most significant
factors.
We found it important to agree on one
major assumption to simplify the tremendous
overload
invariably
present in the
photographic
document and
help us develop
a view that would lead to an agreed
conclusion.
Assumption: |
Education is a communication process-from teacher to student, from student
to teacher, between student and student, and between the student and
himself. |
Corollary: |
From viewing film we cannot tell whether education is
taking place, but
we might be able to tell circumstantially if education could
take place,
and be reasonably sure of the circumstances in which education is not
taking place. |
(An analogy of the visible proof within
the model: If telephone wires are seen
entering
a house
we can assume
that messages
might be sent
or received;
if
telephone wires are dangling, unattached
to the house, we can safely assume
there is no
longer
telephone
communication.)
We studied our footage
as film, only occasionally
studying behavior frame
for frame. Hence
our variables primarily
are in motion,
and much of our
qualified observation was seen in the
sequence of time. Thus time itself
was a variable.
We tried to standardize
nonverbal communication patterns so
that we could recognize
behavioral elements in
a comparable way. The
basic elements
for nonverbal
research in still images are very similar
to those in film, though film offers
special subtleties
visible only
in film.
These tangible
elements
are of three
dimensions:
Birdwhistell’s Kinesics-the
language of posture and gesture
Hall’s Proxemics-the language
of space
Flow of time as a measure of duration
of human performance and the validity
of behavior
seen
only through repetition
in time.
A fourth element is the comparability
between classrooms, which
offers a most important
descriptive opportunity.
Indeed without
the element
of contrast
and
comparison between classes the first
three elements have only limited usefulness.
Comparison of a
large number
of differing
classes was
the core of the analysis of the film
data.
In the BIA school in Kwethluk
students often sit in straight lines.
Film clip
records
the effort
of pre-first
students
to “Stay in your seats!” in
a straight line of chairs. We can observe
they are not physically relating and
form a distracted group. Compare
with film clip of Head Start class.
Examples of Communication in the Classroom
1) TEACHER
SENDING:
Kinesics: |
Gestures of message projection
Eye messages, hand messages, body posture messages
Visual display of artifacts that communicate |
Proxemics: |
Space between teacher and student as a communication variable
Space adjustment as an effort in communication
Extreme proximity for one-to-one projection
Touching the listener, body-to-body communication |
STUDENT RECEIVING:
Kinesics: |
Eye reception, attention, focus
Body reception-turning toward sender, leaning toward sender |
Proxemics: |
Space adjustment to improve reception
Touching the teacher for body-to-body reception |
Time: |
Internalization span of listening
Ability over a rime span to reject interruptions and distractions |
STUDENT SENDING:
Kinesics: |
Signalling to teacher
Hands up; eye signals, speech signals, body signals |
Proxemics: |
Adjustment of space for better projection
Communication by touch |
Time: |
Measure of time span of communication |
TEACHER RECEIVING:
Kinesics: |
Response by eye reception
Leaning forward to listen
Body reception-nodding, head-shaking, hand signal answers |
Proxemics: |
Adjustment in space to heat better
Reception by body touch |
Time: |
Measure of listening span of teacher |
3) STUDENT TO STUDENT
COMMUNICATION:
Kinesics: |
Eye focus for projection and reception
Hand messages, approval, disapproval, etc.
Collaborative reception during lesson or over assignments |
Proxemics: |
Body proximity and body touch signaling
Passing notes, holding our books or other lesson objects
Extra-curricular communication, object sharing, joint activities that link together
a peer culture |
4) STUDENT TO SELF COMMUNICATION:
Programming: |
Social freedom in the classroom that allows the student
to withdraw and think through his own problems and do self-directed study |
Proxemics: |
Student operating in his own air space even within congested
classroom
Images of inner-directed concentration in the classroom |
Time: |
Measure of time freedom offered each student to work our
his own problem or complete his own creative work |
5) VARIABLES OF
MOTIVATION OR BOREDOM:
Positive: |
Coordinated body posture, responding to communication stimulus
Eye behavior suggesting focus of attention
Face signaling expectation and concentration, tonal character of facial muscles
Hand signals of cognition |
Negative: |
Body coordination directed away from stimulus of communication
Body tonus slack and uncoordinated, slouched
Eye behavior suggesting near sleep, daydreaming, out the window
Facial muscles slack and uncoordinated by lack of intellectual or emotional focus |
6) EMOTIONAL
WELL-BEING OR
STRESS:
Well-Being: |
Physical stance of confidence and command over classroom
circumstance
Stance suggesting confidence and command over personality
Body behavior suggesting flow of physical and emotional energy
Eye response and body reactions revealing alertness
Facial and body signals of peaceful adjustment to circumstance and pleasure gained
from classroom environment |
Stress: |
Body tension suggesting fear, withdrawal from classroom
interaction, inability to communicate
Withdrawal suggested by body slumping, head pillowed on arms, etc.
Facial expression of stress, knitted brows, tense lips, suggesting fear, hostility,
or resentment |
With all these considerations in mind each team
member studied every foot of classroom film and logged observations on a scheduled
analysis sheet including:
General impression of class
Asthetic look and tone of the classroom, physical layout
Relationships between staff and child
Relationships between child and child
Relationships between child and staff
Character of these communicational situations: verbal/non-verbal
Is the teacher oriented toward individuals or toward the group?
These scheduled
observations were inventories of what is there in terms of visible
evidence and allowed us to track systematically the imagery to describe
the general shape and effect of Eskimo education.
The researchers worked
alone, each viewing all the footage and listening
to the tapes. The film was looked at without stopping, stopped, run slow-motion,
and clicked through frame for frame for spot analysis of refinement of
behavior. In addition to the schedule of observation, each member wrote
his own general
evaluation, class for class. The consensus was fairly even, and where
there were discrepancies, these classes were restudied.
After the initial
film reading was completed, these data sheets were coded and the material
transferred to key-punch cards which carried our
study
to a further abstraction that defined the educational movement from
the tundra
villages to Bethel and finally to the city of Anchorage.
How did the
sound recording figure in our evaluations?
Because we are a verbally-oriented
people, the team found that if the tapes were played with the film, the verbal
stimuli drained off the
nonverbal sensitivity and made visual reading difficult. Hence
a large part of
the
film analysis
was done with silent film. The tapes, on the other hand, when played
as a check against the silent reading of the film, often deepened
and gave
substance
to
the judgment of the classrooms. In effect the tapes were explanatory but did not prove revealing in themselves.
As author of the report,
I studied all the team’s observations, listened
to the tapes, and rescreened every film. I would move from projector
to typewriter and montage the joint observations on all the classrooms. Chapter
5, “The
classrooms on film,” is this combined statement. Throughout
the research I leaned confidently on the most rewarding character
of photographic data,
the analyst’s opportunity to go back again and again to the
undisturbed raw data. Despite intrusions of subjectivity that invades
even the technology
of photography, I believe the photographic record remains the most
undistorted evidence we have of human behavior.
|